Baby

Babies also listen with their eyes

Did you know that infants who are exposed to a variety of languages can tell the difference between their mother tongue and other languages?

However, this ability fades by the time they reach the age of eight months unless they are familiar with the languages tested.

In auditory studies, they’ve shown that, at birth, babies can tell apart the sounds from all the world’s languages and then over the first year of life, they lose sensitivity to sounds that aren’t part of their mother tongue,” said lead study author Whitney Marie Weikum. “I wanted to see if it was similar for visual clues.

Her study clearly showed that it is. She found that babies can distinguish between languages visually until they are six months old. Bilingual babies are even more capable in that they visually discern their two native languages.

To determine that babies use visual clues, researchers set up silent video clips of three bilingual speakers reciting sentences from The Little Prince in French and English. Babies would sit on a parent’s lap in front of a 27-inch television screen hooked up to a computer. The parents wore dark sunglasses to prevent them from seeing the screen and influencing their child. Each trial consisted of multiple clips presented to the infant for a maximum of 16 seconds. When the baby looked away, an experimenter watching the baby’s face via closed-circuit camera would press a key.

Digital video analysis verified experimenters’ response times. It was assumed that if babies looked at a clip for a longer-than-average period, it was because they noticed the language change. Babies in the control groups would see the same people speaking a single language. Babies in the experimental groups saw the same people using both languages. Researchers then averaged the test times and compared them with the times babies stared at clips in the control and experimental groups.

They tested 96 infants, 24 of whom were exposed to a second language (French) at least 25 % of the time. The others were exposed only to English. Tests were conducted on three groups:  4-, 6- and 8-month-old babies.

To see if babies could visually distinguish their native English from the rhythmically different French, the researchers showed the same people saying different sentences. They found that at 4 and 6 months of age, the experimental group of infants looked significantly longer at clips when the languages switched. By 8 months of age, however, the babies in monolingual environments no longer noticed the switch, while those in bilingual environments continued to be able to tell the difference. “It shows that babies’ abilities match their language environments,” says Weikum.

This study is very important for two reasons,” says Dr. Fred Genesee, a specialist in bilingual research and a professor at McGill University. “It adds scientific evidence that infants have extremely powerful processing capacities that facilitate the challenge of learning languages. It is widely believed that dual language exposure is not simply a challenge, but a burden for infants and can slow down and perhaps even impede normal development. This study shows that this perception underestimates infants’ learning capacity. The additional stimulation serves to maintain infants’ discrimination ability beyond those of monolingual children; this, in turn, lays the foundation for simultaneous bilingual acquisition.”

Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development (CEECD)

The mandate of the CEECD is to foster the dissemination of scientific knowledge on the development of young children with an emphasis, but not exclusively, on the social and emotional development and on the services and policies that influence this development.


This week
Ear infections, antibiotics, and prevention

Becoming a parent also means being acquainted with several small infections encountered during our own childhood. Ear infections are numerous and can leave you having lots of questions. We try to respond to the most frequent ones.

My child is often absentminded!

Do you find yourself often repeating phrases like "Hello? Is anyone there?" ? If so, it seems that your child is often absentminded. Here's how to help your distracted children stay concentrated.

A teenager’s bedroom

Your teenager's bedroom is a disaster. You even invented new words to describe this horrendous place where food and clothes seem to blend into a new kind of carpet but your child doesn't seem to mind. What can you do?

My child is smelly!

Your child is now 6 years old. The innocence of childhood still shines brightly in his or her eyes but… they're smelly! When your child gets hot, you scrunch your nose and smell a tinge of sweat. Are they too young for deodorant?